TESTING MEMO 1: GUESSING ON MULTIPLE-CHOICE TESTS by Robert B. Frary Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University Ever since multiple-choice tests became widely used, in the 1920s, there has been concern over the fact that guessing affects the scores on these tests. At first the phenomenon was not well understood, and score increases due to guessing were uncritically viewed as ill-gotten gains even though these score components usually reflect partial knowledge--the ability to eliminate some wrong choices before guessing. The reaction of some educators was to admonish students against all guessing, directly or indirectly condemning it as dishonest. Of course, admonishing students against guessing was widely ineffective as well as unfair to those who refrained, so long as the tests were scored on the basis of the number of right answers. As a result, many educators avoided use of multiple-choice tests. However, multiple-choice tests became indispensable for mass testing and were found to have other virtues which argued for their inclusion in the educational process, such as broader coverage of instructional topics, accuracy of scoring, and provision of statistical feedback at the item level. Hence, neither admonishment against guessing nor avoidance of multiple-choice tests was a satisfactory approach to resolving what was perceived as a problem. One approach which gained wide acceptance was the use of a scoring formula which "corrects" for purely random guessing. The conventional correction formula subtracts a fraction of the wrong answers from the number-right score. A mathematically equivalent procedure is to award partial score credit for omitted questions rather than deduct score credit for wrong answers. The later approach has a psychological advantage over the former method because it rewards the desired behavior, not guessing completely at random, rather than penalizing undesired behavior. Regardless of which correction formula is employed, it is an ethical requirement that students be encouraged to answer all questions on which one or more choices can be eliminated as incorrect. Only if the answer would represent a sheer guess among all choices should the examinee be advised to omit the question when formula scoring is to be used. Either of the procedures just described may be desirable when many examinees are expected to be unable to finish or to be completely ignorant on large proportions of test questions. However, there are several reasons why either method of "correcting for guessing" is likely to be undesirable in a typical academic setting: 1. Very few examinees will be so ignorant or so slow that they will fail to attempt or be completely unable to eliminate a single wrong choice on any substantial proportion of questions. Hence the effort of "correcting for guessing" is largely wasted. The few who legitimately should omit substantial proportions of questions under formula scoring will be so low in achievement that very low scores will result regardless of whether completely random guessing is suppressed. 2. The admonishment not to guess in the absence of information may be interpreted differently by each examinee and thus may introduce score variance associated with personality or background factors. This phenomenon has been confirmed in numerous published studies. Other published studies have shown that when students do omit questions under conventional "correction for guessing" instructions, they are (on the average) able to choose significantly more correct answers to these questions than under chance expectation. 3. Individuals may choose to disregard the instructions because, on the average, "correcting for guessing" does not penalize for random guessing but only removes the score gain expected from completely random guessing. In fact, if a student's knowledge is inadequate for obtaining a needed score, the best strategy for that student is to guess on all questions, hoping that luck in the short term will be favorable. Because this action is contrary to the instructions not to guess randomly among all choices, the instructor is placed in the questionable position of giving directions which some students may ignore to their benefit. In balance, then, it is difficult to recommend any scoring procedure to control guessing for multiple-choice testing in a typical academic setting. In the absence of this practice, the only fair procedure is to advise all students that it is to their advantage to answer every question regardless of perceived knowledge. For more information, contact Bob Frary at Robert B. Frary, Director of Measurement and Research Services Office of Measurement and Research Services 2096 Derring Hall Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University Blacksburg, VA 24060 703/231-5413 (voice) frary#064;vtvm1.cc.vt.edu ### .